The Man Who Couldn’t Stop (8 page)

BOOK: The Man Who Couldn’t Stop
3.8Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

No, the controversy came because the scientists said the results of the Dunedin psychiatric assessments held some important implications for society. In the official write-up of the study's findings, the researchers suggested that doctors screen the ‘normal' population to identify – and treat – these subclinical obsessive and compulsive symptoms. This might lower the risk of some people going on to develop ‘full-blown' OCD, and other mental health problems, they said, which would reduce distress and costs in the long run. They concluded:

Cost-effectiveness analyses will be required to decide whether these cases should be treated, but such calculations should take into account that treatment of mild cases might prevent a substantial proportion of future serious cases.

Not so fast, said Murray Stein, a psychiatrist at the University of California San Diego. In an editorial published in the same issue of the
American Journal of Psychiatry
as the Dunedin study's results, Stein cautioned against any assumption that people with symptoms of OCD need help if they have not asked for it. ‘We must consider,' he said, ‘the very real possibility that the reason so many people with obsessive-compulsive symptoms fail to get treatment is that they manage quite well.' He continued:

 

As mental health professionals, we should do everything we can to promote awareness about and accessibility to mental health interventions. As clinicians we have an obligation to help reduce the suffering and improve the functioning of the patients who come to us for help. But, lest we forget, most people with obsessive-compulsive symptoms in the community, whether diagnosable or subthreshold or anywhere in between, are not patients. To suggest that we do more to identify and treat such individuals implies that we know better than they whether and when they need our help.

Who needs help? I did, but I didn't accept that I did, at least at first. One problem with OCD is that this ‘at first' stage can last for years. That's down to a mental paradox. On the one hand, the thoughts and fears of OCD blended so seamlessly with the rest of my cognition, they felt so embedded and so real that it was hard to believe they could be taken away. On the other hand, I knew the thoughts were silly. And just like almost everybody, I had other types of silly thoughts too. I felt the urge to jump from a high place. I had random ideas that I had written the wrong name on a birthday card the moment I sealed it inside the envelope. I checked the back door was locked even though I just turned the key. And these intrusive thoughts went away. They went away by themselves. So I thought my thoughts of HIV would also go away by themselves.

I knew that I couldn't catch Aids from someone else using my toothbrush, or from dried blood on the ice rink where someone had burst their nose, or from blood that might be contained in water dripping from an upstairs window that landed in my eye when I looked up. I knew that if I had sat on a drug user's syringe I would have felt it. I knew that my mum's towels were safe, even though she had to have a blood transfusion one Christmas. And so I thought that one morning, hopefully tomorrow morning, I would wake up and leave behind the silly thoughts that made me worried about all of those things. Unfortunately, that's not how OCD works. My intrusive thoughts did go away, but there was a catch. One went away just as soon as another came along to take its place.

It was the US psychologist William James in 1892 who first described thoughts as a ‘stream of consciousness'. The term was subsequently popularized by a writing style in which undirected words seem to flow from an author's head onto the page –
Ulysses
by James Joyce is perhaps the best-known example. Like all streams, this mental flow is uneven – there are fast and slow sections, eddies and currents, pools and falls. Some thoughts relate to current tasks, or those we have just finished or are about to start. Some are triggered by other thoughts or by actions, or as a clear response to external events. Intrusive and obsessive thoughts are different. They seem to bob up from nowhere.

I was a serial monogamist when it came to OCD. My stream of consciousness had a taut net strung across it, a net just big enough to trap one misshapen irrational thought at a time. The only way to free a trapped thought was for another to knock it out, send it on its way, and for it to settle there instead. Some intrusive thoughts would remain for days or weeks, others would last for just a few minutes. But the net was always full. And the content of the net, the thought that was in residence at the time, was always on my mind.

*   *   *

Not everybody experiences OCD in the same way. For some people with OCD the mental hijack of their attention is the only apparent symptom of the disorder. They don't carry out overt compulsions. They don't feel the need to convert their mental anguish into physical form. They say they suffer from a form of OCD called pure-O.

Most of these people, scientists think, do still carry out compulsions – but they are mental compulsions. They might try to ‘undo' the impact of an intrusive thought by deliberately thinking of something else to neutralize it. Or they might start to predict the kind of situations that are likely to trigger the unwanted obsessive thoughts, and then seek to avoid them. Both of those mental routines are compulsions. When someone with OCD uses them to suppress or push away the thought, they reinforce it as surely as if they had performed a physical act like touching a wall or checking for blood on a rusty nail. Their response legitimizes the thought and gives it significance.

For most people, the intrusive thoughts are what bother them and the compulsions are a relief, albeit a temporary one. The intrusive thoughts come first. But some people with OCD describe what sounds like a reversed flow of this cause and effect. The behaviour, the compulsion, is more like a tic. It comes first. They can't explain why they need to tap their hand on their leg a set number of times. They are not doing it as a way to drive something from their mind. They just feel they need to do it, and if they resist the urge to do so then they get anxious.

In these cases the intrusive thoughts follow the resisted compulsion – if these people don't tap their leg then they worry something awful will happen, perhaps their parents will die in a car accident. That's a form of OCD known as the just-not-right experience.

It's pretty difficult to track the true course of events in OCD, the sequence of obsession-compulsion-obsession and where it begins and ends. After all, a circle, even a vicious one, has no beginning or end. But there is evidence that some people with OCD find the unwanted compulsive behaviour distressing, rather than the unwanted obsessive thoughts. Other psychologists have taken this idea further. They have suggested that, even in cases where the compulsions appear to follow intrusive thoughts, it could be the behaviour and not the cognition that triggers the OCD event. The thought appears only as a way to justify the odd behaviour.

The high-place phenomenon – the common urge to jump from a window or bridge – has been explained like this: Say you are standing near the edge of a cliff and enjoying the view. There is no safety barrier and part of your brain, the part that watches for hazards and instinctively avoids them, gets nervous and instructs the legs to take a step back, to minimize the risk. A different part of the brain registers this order to mobilize the legs and seeks to explain it, by comparing the imagined threat to the real threat. Yet there is no immediate threat. You are standing still. So why step back? What's the problem? The brain – the intrusive thought generator – throws up an (irrational) suggestion: the problem must have been that you wanted to jump.

That all happens in milliseconds, and the strongest signal, the one that breaks through from all of this subconscious activity, is the conclusion that comes as an intrusive thought: I want to jump.

*   *   *

OCD dissolves perspective. It magnifies small risks, warps probabilities and takes statistical chance as a prediction, not a sign of how unlikely things are. Example – someone once told me that to catch HIV from a kiss was a one in a million chance. But there are seven billion people in the world, right? And if they all kiss someone at some point in their life, then more than 7,000 of them are at risk. If we assume that only about one in 3,500 kissed people have HIV, then that still leaves two people who will catch the virus that way. Why shouldn't one of them be me?

That's risk assessment by homeopathy. The hazard is so dilute that it is no longer present. Yet as Jim Carrey's character in the film
Dumb and Dumber
replies with excitement when a woman says the odds of her agreeing to join him on a date are one in a million: ‘So, you're telling me there's a chance!' Even when I accepted that it was OCD that made me feel this way, which took a while, there is still the fear of an ironic twist, that someone with an obsessive fear that they will catch HIV in a spectacularly unlikely way could be one of the unfortunate individuals who falls foul of the numbers, and actually does. People
have
contracted HIV in unusual circumstances – from their dentist, for example.

That helps to explain why people with OCD perform the same checks, again and again. We see the one in a million event and not the nine hundred and ninety-nine thousand nine hundred and ninety-nine non-events that we should do. If I touched a door handle with a scratch or a scab on my finger then my mind instantly told me to check there was no blood there. Many times it told me to check before I touched the handle in the first place. At the time, it makes sense. No blood, no risk. To check, I thought, would make my life easier. Each time, I believed that one more, one last, check would give me the certainty I craved. But one check was never enough. Afterwards, each time, I doubted how thoroughly I had completed the check. So I would do it again.

I once stared for an hour at a photograph of me and some friends in a youth hostel in France to try to convince myself that our toothbrushes were different colours, so I would know no one else would have used mine. You could just make out the toothbrushes held in a cup that the photograph showed on the shelf behind us. Well, you needed a magnifying glass really, so I bought one. Each time I put down the picture, the urge to pick it up and look again returned.

For a while, psychologists thought that the repeated checks of OCD might be down to poor memory, and the prior checks simply not remembered. But if there is a memory deficit, then it's a very specific one: people with OCD may not be able to remember clearly if they locked the front door, but they can tell you what they had for breakfast before they left the house. The answer seems more subtle. It is not the accuracy of recall that matters in OCD, but a loss of confidence that those memories are true. People who carry out compulsive checks seem to trust their memory less, and the more they check, the more this distrust grows. It is another vicious cycle: memory uncertainty provokes the need to check, and to check increases memory uncertainty.

Here's an example of that in practice, one that features one of the dullest experiments in the history of science. Psychologists at Concordia University in Montreal asked students to turn on an electric stove, turn it off again, and then check it was off. Others were asked to turn a tap on and off. All were told to repeat their task again and again and again until they had done it nineteen times. Before and after the repetitive tasks, the volunteers (including those allocated to the taps) were each asked to turn off three knobs on the stove – and then to check they were off. Immediately after each test, they were asked to recall which they had turned off.

All of the students could recall the details of the first test. They remembered how they had turned off the stove switches. But, for the second test, the one performed after the repetitive checks, the scientists found some important differences. Those students who had spent all of their time with the stove remembered the second test check differently from the first. They had less confidence that their memory was accurate, and their recall was hazy – not as detailed or vivid. The study showed that repeated checks of the cooker – but not of the taps − introduced doubt in the mind of the checker. Other scientists have repeated the experiment and found the same effect on memory. It can start after just two checks.

The results suggest that the more someone checks that a cooker is off, or that there is no blood on a tissue, then the more that scene becomes familiar. The brain remembers familiar events differently, it tends to focus on meaning rather than colour and shape, which are easier to recall. This makes recall of familiar events less detailed. In OCD this means, the more that we do something, the less sure we can be that we did.

The problem goes deeper. Some people with OCD can stare at a light switch, and still not convince themselves it is off, or look carefully at their scrubbed hands and not believe they are clean. That cannot be down to changes in memory. It is altered perception, and there is some evidence that, just as to check damages confidence in memory, so to stare, even for just a few seconds, reduces confidence in the information collected by the eyes. To reread names, addresses and documents, as people with OCD often must do, could render their meaning less clear − just as to say a word over and over again seems to make it lose its meaning, a well-documented effect known as semantic satiation.

*   *   *

I knew my OCD was bad when I decided to write a poem about it. I don't have the poem any more, and I wouldn't include it if I did. Some things are just too awful to share. The gist of it was that I was a stone and that my friends were fish. They sprouted legs and walked on the land. They grew up. They changed. They evolved. I stayed as a stone. Self-pity is rarely attractive, but even so.

 

FIVE

The OCD family

New York City firefighters call a death-trap dwelling a Collyers Mansion. The nickname comes from the Harlem brownstone house of Homer and Langley Collyer, two brothers who were found dead there in 1947, surrounded by 140 tonnes of collected items and rubbish, including prams, rusted bicycles, old food, potato peelers, guns, glass chandeliers, bowling balls, camera equipment, dressmaker dummies, a canoe, the folding top of a horse-drawn carriage, rusty bed springs, pickled human organs and a two-headed baby in jars, eight live cats, broken Christmas trees, the chassis of a Model T Ford car, fourteen pianos and pots of their own urine and excrement. The brothers were compulsive hoarders. Like most hoarders, they did not see the problem.

BOOK: The Man Who Couldn’t Stop
3.8Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Other books

The Summerland by T. L. Schaefer
the Man from the Broken Hills (1975) by L'amour, Louis - Talon-Chantry
After Dark by Phillip Margolin
The Elder Origins by Bre Faucheux
Table for Two-epub by Jess Dee
The Fantasy by Ryan, Nicole
Midsummer Night's Mayhem by Lauren Quick
El miedo a la libertad by Erich Fromm