In the Land of INVENTED LANGUAGES

BOOK: In the Land of INVENTED LANGUAGES
6.59Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

To Derrick

CONTENTS
 

Nine Hundred Languages, Nine Hundred Years

1.
Scaring the Mundanes

2.
A History of Failure

John Wilkins and the Language of Truth

3.
The Six-Hundred-Page Rewrite

4.
A Calculus of Thought

5.
A Hierarchy of the Universe

6.
The Word for “Shit”

7.
Knowing What You Mean to Say

Ludwik Zamenhof and the Language of Peace

8.
A Linguistic Handshake

9.
Un Nuov Glot

10.
Trouble in Volapükland

11.
A Nudist, a Gay Ornithologist, a Railroad Enthusiast, and a Punk Cannabis Smoker Walk into a Bar …

12.
Crank Pride

Charles Bliss and the Language of Symbols

13.
Word Magic

14.
Hit by a Personality Tornado

15.
Those Queer and Mysterious Chinese Characters

16.
The Spacemen Speak

17.
The Catastrophic Results of Her Ignorance

James Cooke Brown and the Language of Logic

18.
The Whorfian Hypothesis

19.
A Formula for Success

20.
Suitable Apologies

21.
Meaning Quicksand

22.
To Menstruate Joyfully

The Klingons, the Conlangers, and the Art of Language

23.
Flaws or Features?

24.
The Go-To Linguist

25.
What Are They
Doing
?

26.
The Secret Vice

Appendix A: The List of Languages

Appendix B: Language Samples

Notes

Acknowledgments

Scaring
the Mundanes
 

K
lingon speakers, those who have devoted themselves to the study of a language invented for the
Star Trek
franchise, inhabit the lowest possible rung on the geek ladder. Dungeons & Dragons players, ham radio operators, robot engineers, computer programmers, comic book collectors—they all look down on Klingon speakers. Even the most ardent
Star Trek
fanatics, the Trekkies, who dress up in costume every day, who can recite scripts of entire episodes, who collect
Star Trek
paraphernalia with mad devotion, consider Klingon speakers beneath them. When a discussion of Klingon appeared on
Slashdot.org
—the Web site billed as “News for Nerds”—the topic inspired comments like “I'm sorry but it's people like this that give science fiction a bad name.” Another said that Klingon speakers “provide excellent reasons for
forced sterilization. Then again being able to speak Klingon pretty much does this without surgery.”

Mark Shoulson, who has a wife and two children, doesn't enjoy being talked about this way. “It's okay to laugh about it, because it's funny. It's legitimate to laugh. Klingon has entertainment as part of its face value. But I do get annoyed at some of the ruder stuff.” Mark was my unofficial guide to the world of Klingon. When I met him, we lived in the same New Jersey town. I discovered this browsing the Internet, where I also found that he was assistant director of the Klingon Language Institute (KLI) and editor of the Klingon translation of
Hamlet
. I wrote him, and he e-mailed me back the same day, saying he was so excited by the prospect of another Klingon speaker so close by that he didn't even finish reading my message before he responded.

I wasn't yet a Klingon speaker, and I wasn't really planning on becoming one. I was a linguist who had developed a side interest in the subject of artificial languages, and I wanted to talk to Mark for research purposes. People really spoke Klingon—so claimed the Klingon Language Institute materials anyway—and I wasn't sure what that meant. When people “spoke” Klingon, was it playacting? Spitting out little words and phrases and putting on a show? A charades-like guessing game where someone sort of cobbled together a message and someone else sort of understood it? Or was it actual language use?

If it was the latter, then this was something I needed to see for myself, because that would make Klingon something so remarkable as to be almost unheard of—a consciously invented language that had been brought to life.

Although we like to call language mankind's greatest invention, it wasn't invented at all. The languages we speak were not created according to any plan or design. Who invented French? Who
invented Portuguese? No one. They just happened. They arose. Someone said something a certain way, someone else picked up on it, and someone else embellished. A tendency turned into a habit, and somewhere along the way a system came to be. This is how pidgins, slangs, and dialects are born; this is the way English, Russian, and Japanese were born. This is the way all natural languages are born—organically, spontaneously.

The variety of shape, pattern, and color found in the languages of the world is a testament to the wonder of nature, to the breathtaking array of possibilities that can emerge, tangled and wild, from the fertile human endowments of brain and larynx, intelligence and social skills. The job of the linguist, like that of the biologist or the botanist, is not to tell us how nature should behave, or what its creations should look like, but to describe those creations in all their messy glory and try to figure out what they can teach us about life, the world, and, especially in the case of linguistics, the workings of the human mind.

In libraries organized according to the Library of Congress call number system, linguists can usually be found in the stacks classified in the first half of the Ps, anywhere from subclass P, which covers general linguistics, to subclass PN, where “literature” starts. When I was in graduate school, I used to wander this territory, in a procrastinatory haze, noting how the languages covered by the intervening categories became more and more “exotic” the farther I got from PA (Greek and Latin). I would first pass through aisles and aisles of Romance languages, then Germanic, Scandinavian, English, Slavic. There at the end of the Slavic section, at PG9501, things would start to get interesting, with Albanian, followed by the offerings of PH—the Finno-Ugrics (Veps, Estonian, Udmurt, Hungarian), the mysterious Basque. By the time I got to PL, I would be far from Europe, drifting through
Asia and Africa, lingering over A
Grammar of the Hoava Language, Western Solomons
or
The Southern Bauchi Group of Chadic Languages: A Survey Report
.

The final subclass, PM, was a tour through the New World, starting with the Eskimo languages of Greenland and Alaska and proceeding southward through Tlingit, Kickapoo, and Navajo to the Mayan and Aztecan languages of Mexico and Central America, down across the Amazon, through the Andes and the plains of Brazil, until I reached the islands off the southernmost tip of South America with
Yámana-English: A Dictionary of the Speech of Tierra del Fuego
. From there, there was nowhere to go but to the borders of language itself—the contact, or “mixed,” languages, the pidgins and creoles of the PM7800s:
Spanish Contact Vernaculars in the Philippine Islands; Le Créole de Breaux Bridge, Louisiane: étude morphosyntaxique, textes, vocabulaire
.

At the very end of this lush orchid garden of languages there was one more section, where linguists don't generally care to visit—a few lonely shelves of faded plastic flowers, the artificial languages.
The Klingon Dictionary
was here, among other books on languages I had never heard of: Babm, aUI, Nal Bino, Leno Gi-Nasu, Tutonish, Ehmay Ghee Chah. These were not lighthearted language games, like pig Latin, or the spontaneous results of in-group communication, like Cockney rhyming slang or surfer jargon. They were invented on purpose, cut from whole cloth, set down on paper, start to finish, by one person. They had chapters and chapters of grammar and extensive dictionaries. They were testaments not to the wonder of nature but to the human impulse to master nature. They were deliberate, painstakingly crafted attempts to tame language by making it more orderly, more rational, less burdened with inconsistencies and irregularities. There were
hundreds of them. And they were all failures, dead in the water, spoken by no one.

Other books

Killer Secrets by Katie Reus
Witness the Dead by Craig Robertson
Dead Man Dancing by Marcia Talley
Recipes for Melissa by Teresa Driscoll
Cruiser by Dee J. Stone
The Mysterious Rider by Grey, Zane
Taking In Strays by Kracken
Texas Takedown by Barb Han