Read Incognito Online

Authors: David Eagleman

Incognito (36 page)

BOOK: Incognito
13.38Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

45
Grossberg, “How does a brain … ?”; Mumford, “On the computational
architecture”; Ullman, “Sequence seeking”; and Rao, “An optimal estimation approach.”

46
MacKay, “The epistemological problem.”

47
See Blakemore, Wolpert, and Frith, “Why can’t you tickle yourself?” for more about tickling. More generally, violations of sensory expectations can inform a brain about responsibility—that is, did I cause the action or did someone else? Schizophrenic hallucinations may arise from a failure of matching expectations about one’s own motor acts to their resulting sensory signals. Failure to distinguish one’s own actions from those of independent agents means that the patient attributes his internal voices to someone else. For more on this idea, see Frith and Dolan, “Brain mechanisms.”

48
Symonds and MacKenzie, “Bilateral loss of vision.”

49
Eagleman and Sejnowski, “Motion integration,” and Eagleman, “Human time perception.”

50
Eagleman and Pariyadath, “Is subjective duration … ?”

Chapter 3. Mind: The Gap
 

  
1
Macuga, et al., “Changing lanes.”

  
2
Schacter, “Implicit memory.”

  
3
Ebbinghaus,
Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology
.

  
4
Horsey,
The Art of Chicken Sexing
; Biederman and Shiffrar, “Sexing day-old chicks”; Brandom, “Insights and blindspots of reliabilism”; and Harnad, “Experimental analysis.”

  
5
Allan, “Learning perceptual skills.”

  
6
Cohen, Eichenbaum, Deacedo, and Corkin, “Different memory systems,” and Brooks and Baddeley, “What can amnesic patients learn?”

  
7
As another example of tying things together at an unconscious level, subjects were given a carbonated drink, and then their chairs were rocked back and forth to induce motion sickness. As a result, the subjects established an aversion to the carbonated drink, even though they well knew (consciously) that the drink had nothing to do with the nauseating motion. See Arwas, Rolnick, and Lubow, “Conditioned taste aversion.”

  
8
Greenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz, “Measuring individual differences.”

  
9
The implicit association test can be taken online:
https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/demo/selectatest.html
.

10
Wojnowicz, Ferguson, Dale, and Spivey, “The self-organization of explicit attitudes.” See also Freeman, Ambady, Rule, and Johnson, “Will a category cue attract you?”

11
Jones, Pelham, Carvallo, and Mirenberg, “How do I love thee?”

12
Ibid.

13
Pelham, Mirenberg, and Jones, “Why Susie sells,” and Pelham, Carvallo, and Jones, “Implicit egotism.”

14
Abel, “Influence of names.”

15
Jacoby and Witherspoon, “Remembering without awareness.”

16
Tulving, Schacter, and Stark, “Priming effects.” These effects hold even if I distract you so much that we’re certain you cannot explicitly remember what the words were; you’re still just as good at the word completion. See Graf and Schacter, “Selective effects.”

17
The idea of priming has a rich history in literature and entertainment. In
The Subliminal Man
, by J. G. Ballard (1963), a character named Hathaway is the only one who suspects that the dozens of gigantic blank signs towering over the roads are really subliminal advertising machines, encouraging people to take on more jobs and buy more products. A more droll incarnation of Subliminal Man can be found in comedian Kevin Nealon’s
Saturday Night Live
character, who says, during a talk show interview, “I’ve always liked watching this show (nauseating). It’s fun to be a guest on this show (torture). It’s kind of like a second home to me (
Titanic
).”

18
Graf and Schacter, “Implicit and explicit memory.”

19
See Tom, Nelson, Srzentic, and King, “Mere exposure.” For a more basic approach to demonstrating that the brain can absorb what it has seen even without attention to it, see Gutnisky, Hansen, Iliescu, and Dragoi, “Attention alters visual plasticity.”

20
Ironically, no one is quite sure who said this first. The quotation has been variously attributed to Mae West, P. T. Barnum, George M. Cohan, Will Rogers, and W. C. Fields, among others.

21
Hasher, Goldstein, and Toppino, “Frequency and the conference of referential validity.”

22
Begg, Anas, and Farinacci, “Dissociation of processes in belief.”

23
Cleeremans,
Mechanisms of Implicit Learning
.

24
Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, and Damasio, “Deciding advantageously.”

25
Damasio, “The somatic marker hypothesis”; Damasio,
Descartes’ Error
; and Damasio,
The Feeling of What Happens.

26
Eagleman,
Live-Wired
.

27
Montague,
Your Brain Is (Almost) Perfect
.

28
If you watch athletes closely, you will notice they often employ physical rituals to get themselves into the zone—for example, dribbling the ball exactly three times, cranking their neck to the left, and then shooting. By providing predictability, these rituals ease them into a less conscious state. To the same end, repetitive and predictable rituals are routinely used in religious services—for example, rote prayers, rosary counting, and chants all help to relax the buzzing of the conscious mind.

Chapter 4. The Kinds of Thoughts That Are Thinkable
 

  
1
Blaise Pascal,
Pensées
, 1670.

  
2
All of these signals (radio, microwave, X-ray, gamma ray, cell phone transmissions, television broadcasts, and so on) are exactly the same thing as the stuff coming out of the front of your flashlight—just of a different wavelength. Some readers already knew this; for those who didn’t, the sheer amazingness of this simple scientific fact urges its inclusion.

  
3
Jakob von Uexküll introduced the idea of the umwelt in 1909 and explored it through the 1940s. It was then lost for decades until rediscovery and revivification by the semiotician Thomas A. Sebeok in 1979; Jakob von Uexküll, “A stroll through the worlds of animals and men.” See also Giorgio Agamben, Chapter 10, “Umwelt”, in his book
The Open: Man and Animal
, trans. Kevin Attell (Palo Alto: Stanford University Press, 2004); originally published in Italian in 2002 as
L’aperto: l’uomo e l’animale
.

  
4
K. A. Jameson, S. Highnote, and L. Wasserman, “Richer color
experience in observers with multiple photopigment opsin genes,”
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
, 8, no.2 (2001): 244–61; and Jameson, “Tetrachromatic color vision.”

  
5
For more about synesthesia, see Cytowic and Eagleman,
Wednesday Is Indigo Blue
.

  
6
Think you might have synesthesia? Take the free online tests at
www.synesthete.org
. See Eagleman, et al., “A standardized test battery for the study of synesthesia.”

  
7
Our laboratory has turned to the details of synesthesia—from behavior to neuroimaging to genetics—to use it as an inroad into understanding how slight differences in the brain can lead to large differences in the perception of reality. See
www.synesthete.org
.

  
8
In other words, the forms have a location in mental space that can be pointed to. If you’re not a spatial sequence synesthete, imagine your car parked in the space in front of you. Although you do not physically see it there like a hallucination, you will have no trouble pointing to the front wheel, the driver’s side window, the rear bumper, and so on. The car has three-dimensional coordinates in your mental space. So it goes with automatically triggered number forms. Unlike hallucinations, they do not overlie the outside visual world; they instead live in a mental space. In fact, even blind subjects can experience number form synesthesia; see Wheeler and Cutsforth, “The number forms of a blind subject.” For a larger discussion of spatial sequence synesthesia, see Eagleman, “The objectification of overlearned sequences,” and Cytowic and Eagleman,
Wednesday Is Indigo Blue
.

  
9
Eagleman, “The objectification of overlearned sequences.”

10
An interesting speculation is that all brains are synesthetic––but the majority of us remain unconscious of the sensory fusions going on in our brains under the surface of awareness. In fact, everyone seems to possess implicit number lines for sequences. When asked, we might agree that the number line for integers increases as one goes from left to right. Spatial sequence synesthetes differ in that they experience sequences explicitly in three dimensions as automatic, consistent, and concrete configurations. See Eagleman, “The objectification of overlearned
sequences”, and Cytowic and Eagleman,
Wednesday Is Indigo Blue
.

11
Nagel,
The View from Nowhere
.

12
See Cosmides and Tooby,
Cognitive Adaptations
, for an overview, and Steven Pinker’s
The Blank Slate
for an excellent in-depth read.

13
Johnson and Morton, “CONSPEC and CONLERN.”

14
Meltzoff, “Understanding the intentions of others.”

15
Pinker,
The Blank Slate
.

16
Wason and Shapiro, “Reasoning,” and Wason, “Natural and contrived experience”

17
Cosmides and Tooby,
Cognitive Adaptions
.

18
Barkow, Cosmides, and Tooby,
The Adapted Mind

19
Cosmides and Tooby, “Evolutionary psychology: A primer,” 1997;
http://www.psych.ucsb.edu/research/cep/primer.html

20
James,
The Principles of Psychology
.

21
Tooby and Cosmides,
Evolutionary Psychology: Foundational Papers
(Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2000).

22
Singh, “Adaptive significance” and “Is thin really beautiful,” and Yu and Shepard, “Is beauty in the eye?”

23
More generally, women with waists thinner than this range are seen as more aggressive and ambitious, while those with thicker waists are viewed as kind and faithful.

24
Ramachandran, “Why do gentlemen …?”

25
Penton-Voak, et al., “Female preference for male faces changes cyclically.”

26
Vaughn and Eagleman, “Faces briefly glimpsed.”

27
Friedman, McCarthy, Förster, and Denzler, “Automatic effects.” It may even be the case that other concepts related to alcohol (such as sociability) may also be activated by priming to alcohol-related words—such that merely seeing (not drinking) a glass of wine could lead to easier conversation and more eye contact. A more speculative and challenging possibility is that seeing advertisements for alcohol along highway billboards could lead to diminished driving performance.

28
Concealed ovulation (as well as internal fertilization, as opposed to the
laying of external eggs) may have come about as a mechanism that encourages males to remain attentive to their female mates equally at all times, thereby diminishing the chances of desertion.

29
Roberts, Havlicek, and Flegr, “Female facial attractiveness increases.”

30
Symmetry of ears, breasts, and fingers during ovulation: Manning, Scutt, Whitehouse, Leinster, and Walton, “Asymmetry,” Scutt and Manning, “Symmetry”; for lighter skin tone, see Van den Berghe and Frost, “Skin color preference.”

BOOK: Incognito
13.38Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Other books

My Reckless Surrender by Anna Campbell
Enticed by J.A. Belfield
Coal Black Heart by John Demont
Lost Cause by John Wilson
Exodus 2022 by Kenneth G. Bennett
Montana Morning by Sharon Flesch
The Sorcerer's Quest by Rain Oxford
Nine Lives: A Lily Dale Mystery by Wendy Corsi Staub