Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind (4 page)

Read Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind Online

Authors: V. S. Ramachandran,Sandra Blakeslee

Tags: #Medical, #Neurology, #Neuroscience

BOOK: Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind
8.39Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

Another teacher of mine, a professor of neurology, would insist on our diagnosing Parkinson's disease with our eyes closed—by simply listening to the patients' footsteps (patients with this disorder have a characteristic shuffling gait). This detectivelike aspect of clinical medicine is a dying art in this age of high−tech medicine, but it planted a seed in my mind. By carefully observing, listening, touching and, yes, even smelling the patient, one can arrive at a reasonable diagnosis and merely use laboratory tests to confirm what is already known. Finally, when studying and treating a patient, it is the physician's duty always to ask himself, "What does
it feel like
to be in the patient's shoes?" "What if I were?" In doing this, I have never ceased to be amazed at the courage and fortitude of many of my patients or by the fact that, ironically, tragedy itself can sometimes enrich a patient's life and give it new meaning. For this reason, even though many of the clinical tales you will hear are tinged with sadness, equally often they are stories of the triumph of the human spirit over adversity, and there is a strong undercurrent of optimism. For example, one patient I saw—a neurologist from New York—suddenly at the age of sixty started experiencing epileptic seizures arising from his right temporal lobe.

The seizures were alarming, of course, but to his amazement and delight he found himself becoming fascinated by poetry, for the first time in his life. In fact, he began thinking in verse, producing a voluminous outflow of rhyme. He said that such a poetic view gave him a new lease on life, a fresh start just when he was starting to feel a bit jaded. Does it follow from this example that all of us are unfulfilled poets, as many new age gurus and mystics assert? Do we each have an untapped potential for beautiful verse and rhyme hidden in the recesses of our right hemisphere? If so, is there any way we can unleash this latent ability, short of having seizures?

Before we meet the patients, crack mysteries and speculate about brain organization, I'd like to take you on a short guided tour of the human brain. These anatomical signposts, which I promise to keep simple, will help you understand the many new explanations for why neurological patients act the way they do.

It's almost a cliché these days to say that the human brain is the most Figure 1.1

complexly organized form of matter in the universe, and there is actually some truth to this. If you snip away a section of brain, say, from the convoluted outer layer called the neocortex and peer at it under a microscope, 12

you'll see that it is composed of neurons or nerve cells—the basic functional units of the nervous system, where information is exchanged. At birth, the typical brain probably contains over one hundred billion neurons, whose number slowly diminishes with age.

Each neuron has a cell body and tens of thousands of tiny branches called dendrites, which receive information from other neurons. Each neuron also has a primary axon (a projection that can travel long distances in the brain) for sending data out of the cell, and axon terminals for communication with other cells.

If you look at Figure 1.1, you'll notice that neurons make contacts with other neurons, at points called synapses. Each neuron makes anywhere from a thousand to ten thousand synapses with other neurons. These can be either on or off, excitatory or inhibitory. That is, some synapses turn on the juice to fire things up, whereas others release juices that calm everything down, in an ongoing dance of staggering complexity. A piece of your brain the size of a grain of sand would contain one hundred thousand neurons, two million axons and one billion synapses, all "talking to" each other. Given these figures, it's been calculated that the number of possible brain states—the number of permutations and combinations of activity that are theoretically possible—exceeds the number of elementary particles in the universe. Given this complexity, Figure 1.2
Gross anatomy of the human brain, (a) Shows the left side of the left hemisphere. Notice the four
lobes: frontal, parietal, temporal and occipital. The frontal is separated from the parietal by the central or
rolandic sulcus (furrow or fissure), and the temporal from the parietal by the lateral or sylvian fissure, (b)
Shows the inner surface of the left hemisphere. Notice the conspicuous corpus cal−losum (black) and the
thalamus (white) in the middle. The corpus callosum bridges the two hemispheres, (c) Shows the two
hemispheres of the brain viewed down the top.
(a) Ramachandran; (b) and (c) redrawn from Zeki, 1993.

how do we begin to understand the functions of the brain? Obviously, understanding the
structure
of the nervous system is vital to understanding its functions5—and so I will begin with a brief survey of the anatomy of the brain, which, for our purposes here, begins at the top of the spinal cord. This region, called the medulla oblongata, connects the spinal cord to the brain and contains clusters of cells or nuclei that control critical functions like blood pressure, heart rate and breathing. The medulla connects to the pons (a kind of bulge), which sends fibers into the cerebellum, a fist−sized structure at the back of the brain that helps you carry out coordinated movements. Atop these are the two enormous cerebral hemispheres—the famous walnut−shaped halves of the brain. Each half is divided into four lobes—frontal, temporal, parietal and occipital—that you will learn much more about in coming chapters (Figure 1.2).

13

Each hemisphere controls the movements of the muscles (for example, those in your arm and leg) on the opposite side of your body. Your right brain makes your left arm wave and your left brain allows your right leg

to kick a ball. The two halves of the brain are connected by a band of fibers called the corpus callosum. When this band is cut, the two sides can no longer communicate; the result is a syndrome that offers insight into the role each side plays in cognition. The outer part of each hemisphere is composed of cerebral cortex: a thin, convoluted sheet of cells, six layers thick, that is scrunched into ridges and furrows like a cauliflower and packed densely inside the skull.

Right in the center of the brain is the thalamus. It is thought to be evolutionarily more primitive than the cerebral cortex and is often described as a "relay station" because all sensory information except smell passes through it before reaching the outer cortical mantle. Interposed between the thalamus and the cortex are more nuclei, called basal ganglia (with names like the putamen and caudate nucleus). Finally, on the floor of the thalamus is the hypothalamus, which seems to be concerned with regulating metabolic functions, hormone production, and various basic drives such as aggression, fear, and sexuality.

These anatomical facts have been known for a long time, but we still have no clear idea of how the brain works.6 Many older theories fall into two warring camps—modularity and holism—and the pendulum has swung back and forth between these two extreme points of view for the last three hundred years. At one end of the spectrum are modularists, who believe that different parts of the brain are highly specialized for mental capacities. Thus there is a module for language, one for memory, one for math ability, one for face recognition and maybe even one for detecting people who cheat. Moreover, they argue, these modules or regions are largely autonomous. Each does its own job, set of computations, or whatever, and then—like a bucket brigade—passes its output to the next module in line, not "talking" much to other regions.

At the other end of the spectrum we have "holism," a theoretical approach that overlaps with what these days is called "connectionism." This school of thought argues that the brain functions as a whole and that any one part is as good as any other part. The holistic view is defended by th fact that many areas, especially cortical regions, can be recruited for multiple tasks. Everything is connected to everything else, say the holists, and so the search for distinct modules is a waste of time.

My own work with patients suggests that these two points of view are not mutually exclusive—that the brain is a dynamic structure that employs both "modes" in a marvelously complex interplay. The grandeur of the human potential is visible only when we take all the possibilities into account, resisting the temptation to fall into polarized camps or to

ask whether a given function is localized or not localized.7 As we shall see, it's much more useful to tackle each problem as it comes along and not get hung up taking sides.

Each view in its extreme form is in fact rather absurd. As an analogy, suppose you are watching the program
Baywatch
on television. Where is
Baywatch
localized? Is it in the phosphor glowing on the TV screen or in the dancing electrons inside the cathode−ray tube? Is it in the electromagnetic waves being transmitted through air? Or is it on the celluloid film or video tape in the studio from which the show is being transmitted?

Or maybe it's in the camera that's looking at the actors in the scene?

Most people recognize right away that this is a meaningless question. You might be tempted to conclude therefore that
Baywatch
is not localized (there is no
Baywatch
"module") in any one place—that it permeates the whole universe—but that, too, is absurd. For we know it is
not
localized on the moon or in my pet cat or in the chair I'm sitting on (even though some of the electromagnetic waves may reach these locations). Clearly the phosphor, the cathode−ray tube, the electromagnetic waves and the celluloid or tape are all much more 14

directly involved in this scenario we call
Baywatch
than is the moon, a chair or my cat.

This example illustrates that once you understand what a television program really is, the question "Is it localized or not localized?" recedes into the background, replaced with the question "How does it work?" But it's also clear that looking at the cathode−ray tube and electron gun may eventually give you hints about how the television set works and picks up the
Baywatch
program as it is aired, whereas examining the chair you are sitting on never will. So localization is not a bad place to start, so long as we avoid the pitfall of thinking that it holds all the answers.

So it is with many of the currently debated issues concerning brain function. Is language localized? Is color vision? Laughter? Once we understand these functions better, the question of "where" becomes less important than the question of "how." As it now stands, a wealth of empirical evidence supports the idea that there are indeed specialized parts or modules of the brain for various mental capacities. But the real secret to understanding the brain lies not only in unraveling the structure and function of each module but in discovering how they interact with each other to generate the whole spectrum of abilities that we call human nature.

Here is where the patients with bizarre neurological conditions come into the picture. Like the anomalous behavior of the dog that did not

bark when the crime was being committed, providing Sherlock Holmes with a clue as to who might have entered the house on the night of the murder, the odd behavior of these patients can help us solve the mystery of how various parts of the brain create a useful representation of the external world and generate the illusion of a "self" that endures in space and time.

To help you get a feel for this way of doing science, consider these colorful cases—and the lessons drawn from them—taken from the older neurological literature.

More than fifty years ago a middle−aged woman walked into the clinic of Kurt Goldstein, a world−renowned neurologist with keen diagnostic skills. The woman appeared normal and conversed fluently; indeed, nothing was obviously wrong with her. But she had one extraordinary complaint—every now and then her left hand would fly up to her throat and try to strangle her. She often had to use her right hand to wrestle the left hand under control, pushing it down to her side—much like Peter Sellers portraying Dr. Strangelove. She sometimes even had to sit on the murderous hand, so intent was it on trying to end her life.

Not surprisingly, the woman's primary physician decided she was mentally disturbed or hysterical and sent her to several psychiatrists for treatment. When they couldn't help, she was dispatched to Dr. Goldstein, who had a reputation for diagnosing difficult cases. After Goldstein examined her, he established to his satisfaction that she was not psychotic, mentally disturbed or hysterical. She had no obvious neurological deficits such as paralysis or exaggerated reflexes. But he soon came up with an explanation for her behavior: Like you and me, the woman had two cerebral hemispheres, each of which is specialized for different mental capacities and controls movements on the opposite side of the body. The two hemispheres are connected by a band of fibers called the corpus callosum that allows the two sides to communicate and stay "in sync." But unlike most of ours, this woman's right hemisphere (which controlled her left hand) seemed to have some latent suicidal tendencies—a genuine urge to kill herself. Initially these urges may have been held in check by

Other books

The Story Keeper by Lisa Wingate
Good with His Hands by Tanya Michaels
A Spanish Awakening by Kim Lawrence
Gluttony: A Dictionary for the Indulgent by Adams Media Corporation
Mended by Clayborne, By Kimberly M.
The Breakers Code by Conner Kressley
Alan Turing: The Enigma by Andrew Hodges
Click to Subscribe by L. M. Augustine