Inventing Iron Man (16 page)

Read Inventing Iron Man Online

Authors: E. Paul Zehr

BOOK: Inventing Iron Man
9.9Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

Let's begin first with the sensory maps we have. Think about the connection between neurons in the somatosensory cortex and receptors on your index finger. If there is a tragic and traumatic accident and a limb had to be amputated, there would no longer be an index finger with receptors to be connected to the nervous system and
brain. However, the neurons in the somatosensory cortex still do exist and are expecting and awaiting information from the body. There can be an “expansion” of the maps such that areas that were nearby the now-disconnected brain regions take over and make use of the neurons in the old area. This means that there is remodeling of the cortical maps and is a useful response to accommodate the needs of the nervous system. Your nervous system doesn't like to have a gap in the map of the relationship between the body and the brain.

One odd—but not uncommon—effect of missing limbs is phantom limb and the related phantom pain syndrome. Sometimes the representation in the brain of the amputated limb does not fade away and instead gets taken over by some other areas and persists despite the limb amputation—enter the phantom limb. People suffering from phantom limb syndromes can have the distinct sensation that the limb exists and is there. They can feel itching and tingling and perceive weight in the limb, the exact opposite feeling of a leg that has fallen asleep. Phantom limb syndrome would be merely a nuisance if it were all there was to losing a limb. Unfortunately, what often goes along with it is a phantom pain syndrome. This means exactly what it sounds like: the person feels painful sensations that seem to be coming from a limb that no longer exists! As you might guess, this is a very troubling thing to experience.

How can you treat pain in a limb that doesn't physically exist? The work of Vilayanur Ramachandran and colleagues at the University of California at San Diego has taken an interesting approach to this—by tricking the brain into thinking the missing body part does exist. If a split mirror is set up (often a “mirror box” is used), someone sees the other side of their body on the mirror side.
Figure 6.1
shows me using a mirror box at the lab of my friend Richard Carson at Queen's University Belfast in Northern Ireland. Notice that it appears from the reflections that I have two arms, but one is the reflection of the other. My other arm is hidden behind the mirror. This has been used with amputees with phantom limb syndromes, including phantom limb pain, to treat the symptoms. If participants carefully study their movements and do different tasks with the intact limb while looking in the mirror, they will perceive that the amputated limb is actually moving and feeling sensation. The best look at this is in panel C in the figure.

I can tell you that it really did appear to me that I was staring at my right arm even though it was only the reflection of my left. It is a very powerful illusion. (Of course, in a real scenario, I would have been asked to remove my watch from my left wrist. It kind of takes away from the effect.) Using something like this, over time, the sensation of the phantom limb will often be reduced or disappear. This may seem pretty wild, but it appears to be grounded in the fact that of all our senses, our nervous system puts the most weight on vision. If vision informs us that something is happening, we almost always “believe” that over information from other systems. So, in this way the visual system can be used to trick the brain into realizing that there isn't any phantom limb to feel phantom pain.

Figure 6.1. (
opposite
) The author using a “mirror box” apparatus shown from three different perspectives. Notice the appearance from the reflections creates an illusion that I basically have two arms but one is the reflection of the other. My other arm is hidden behind the mirror. This can be best appreciated from panel C. Courtesy Richard Carson.

Tony experienced something similar to this, described in “This Year's Model” (Invincible Iron Man #290, 1993). In this story, Tony Stark underwent a procedure to help “fix” his nervous system degeneration. One of the outcomes of this procedure—which involved a much closer link to his armor—was enhanced sensation. While lying in bed in the recovery room, Tony remarks that “there are additional … side effects. I'm experiencing alterations in perception. Everything is too sharp, too clear. Sights, sounds, textures are overwhelming.” There is also the possibility of creating other weird outcomes associated with certain neurological syndromes such as “synaesthesia,” which is also known as sensory substitution. This means you get a different sensation than you expect coming from some input. An extreme example would be if you could taste words instead of hearing them. In Part 6 of the 2007 Hypervelocity story arc, Tony described experiencing something similar, “a sensory backwash of synaesthesia hits one splinterself—as I taste and touch and smell unlocked datafiles.” The closest story that hints at phantom pain (or probably more of a neuralgia—pain from inflamed nerves) was found in the
Ultimate Iron Man
2006 graphic novel written by novelist Orson Scott Card. In this arc Tony is shown as having a skin condition that resulted in almost constant pain. Sadly, as part of the recurring theme of alcohol use and abuse in Tony Stark's life, he found
that alcohol consumption reduced the painful sensations. But at considerable cost—as we will delve deeply into in the next chapter.

Is There Space in Shellhead's Brain to Store a Skin of Iron?

What would happen if we tried to dramatically increase the representation of the body in the brain without subtracting something? We would need to know this to determine the feasibility of fully integrating a nervous system with an Iron Man suit of armor. We don't have any Iron Man acolytes to bring into the lab to study and get the definitive answer on this. Clearly we can predict that dramatic changes in how the nervous system works would result from connection to the neural interface with the Iron Man suit. The tantalizing question is whether the cost of this adaptation is possibly the ability to use the body. That is, will the connections for the suit be so strong that they replace the normal representation of the body? I think the answer has to be yes. If you take this far enough, it suggests that Tony's protracted use of a suit of armor could lead to his inability to control his human body. Based on what we discussed above and how finely tuned our nervous systems are throughout our lives, Tony Stark's bodily control would be very robust. However, given the extreme nature of the kind of implant needed to connect with the nervous system, a direct brain-machine interface would certainly be the kind of—very unnatural—scenario in which this could be overridden.

Some recent work by Karunesh Ganguly and Jose Carmena at the University of California in Berkeley has addressed a related point. They were interested in getting at what sort of long-term changes in the brain might occur with the use of a prosthetic limb in a brain-computer interface. Recordings were made from neurons in the motor cortex of a monkey while it performed a task that involved reaching toward a target. Each session involved recording from up to a hundred neurons (upper motoneurons) from the motor cortex of each monkey. The researchers then watched what kind of activity occurred when the monkeys did a very simple reach.

To get a basic idea of what the monkeys did, imagine lifting your elbow straight out from the side while keeping your hand at the same height as your elbow. (Yes, lots of these kinds of experiments involve arm movements like this. Partly this is because they are easier to control. Partly it is because the way the motor maps of the brain are
oriented, it is easier to get at the cells for the upper limb.) Now place your hand out in front of your body and imagine straightening your arm out and flexing your arm to bring your hand back. You could try reaching to different places on a flat space right in front of your chest (a two-dimensional, side-to-side movement). These simple kinds of movement are the ones that the monkeys were trained to do by looking at a cursor that would light up to indicate to them what direction to move to next. They would then repeat these trials each day over a two- to three-hour training period for almost three weeks. During the reaches, the electrical activity of the neurons in the motor cortex was recorded.

From these recordings, it was possible to build a map of activity related to the direction that the arm moved. This makes sense, because the neurons were from areas of the brain controlling muscles of the arm. They should be active during reaching with the arm. In fact, the neurons are specially “tuned” (meaning they fire at higher rates) to certain movement directions, and this information can be detected from the brain and used to determine what the brain is trying to do with the arm. Normally, recordings like this would just give information about the output of the motor cortex. However, if you recorded that information with a specialized computer program, you could use it as a control signal for something else. Like a robot arm, for example. Or an Iron Man suit, eventually. At this stage, though, Ganguly and Carmena used this signal to control a computer display of cursors for where the monkey arm would be if it actually moved. This may start to get a little confusing.

The image at the top of
figure 6.2
shows how the monkey moved its arm using the brain-controlled cursor. That means the cursor was moving based only on the commands from the brain—without arm movement itself. Think about this carefully—this means the monkey was controlling the computer cursor to move using a command that would normally come from doing the same movement with its arm. The monkeys were so well trained that they knew what to do to move their arms when a light appeared and generated the same brain activity to move the arm even though the arm itself didn't move anywhere. This is full brain control that is exactly reminiscent of what would be needed for an Iron Man interface to work directly into the nervous system.

In the top panel of
figure 6.2
are data from Ganguly and Carmena's monkey study. Taking this idea toward the long-term effect
of using the Iron Man suit of armor as a brain-machine interface is shown in panels B and C. The motor cortical map we saw in
chapter 3
is redrawn here (panel B) so you can clearly see that integrating the exoskeletal suit of Iron Man armor into the brain means putting it on top of a normally “full” map! Panel C shows figuratively where the Iron Man suit might be represented in the brain.

Figure 6.2. Continued use of a neuroprosthetic (like the Iron Man suit of armor) will lead to plastic changes in the cortical maps of the body. Changes in brain activity shift when a monkey learns a reaching task (
A
). The human motor body map, raising the question where would the Iron Man suit of armor go? (
B
). How the Iron Man suit of armor would have to be somehow incorporated into the normal body map (
C
). Panel A courtesy Sedwick (2009); panel B modified from Penfield and Rasmussen (1950); panel C courtesy Patrick J. Lynch.

It is an open question about how good could the brain representation of the neuroprosthetic actually become. Could it become strong enough and stable enough to become a real memory or an “engram” of a new map? This was the main meat of the Ganguly and Carmena study, and the answer seems to be yes. With practice over almost three weeks of training, the monkeys seemed to form stable cortical maps for the use of the prosthetic. It is surprising (to me at least!) that the neural activity seemed to be easily recalled and used, very stable, and very robust. These are the same features that would normally be described when discussing long-term memories. In fact, they use the term “prosthetic motor memory” to describe this outcome. Ganguly and Carmena go on to predict that with “improvements in technology neuroprosthetic devices could be controlled through effortless recall of such a motor memory in a manner that mimics the natural process of skill acquisition and motor control.” These may well turn out to be prophetic words as we move technologically closer to a true Tony Stark / Iron Man neural interface. This could bring us face to face with the idea of embodiment of an entirely new body.

Other books

Shadow Train by J. Gabriel Gates
The Dog Who Wouldn't Be by Farley Mowat
LUST by Laura B. Cooper
The Osiris Ritual by George Mann
To Kill a President by By Marc james
Life of the Party by Gillian Philip
Natural Lust by Madison Sevier
The Revenge of Geography by Robert D. Kaplan