Read The Domesticated Brain Online

Authors: Bruce Hood

Tags: #Science, #Life Sciences, #Neuroscience

The Domesticated Brain (10 page)

BOOK: The Domesticated Brain
13.87Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

When infants are learning words from an adult for things they have never encountered before, they listen out for the new name but also monitor where the adult is looking. In one study they were shown a new object and when they were looking at it the experimenter said ‘Look at the toopa’ but at the same time was herself looking into a bucket.
40
None of the children associated the word ‘toopa’ with the object they were holding. Children understand new words refer to new things but only those that are introduced in the context of shared joint attention.

By their first birthday, babies are constantly monitoring the faces of others, looking for information, and have even
mastered the skill of pointing that can alert another to something of interest. Initially, babies point because they want something out of reach. Many primates raised in captivity do this as well, though it is more of an open-handed gesture to receive food. Apes also lack the hand anatomy that allows them to extend the index finger in the same way that humans do. However, only human infants will point to things out of sheer interest.
41
Sometimes this is done to solicit a response from an adult, but more often than not the youngster is simply pointing out something interesting to be shared. No other animal does this.
42

Copycats

In addition to joint signalling, we also copy each other. Initially, parents and babies enjoy copying each other’s expressions and noises in reciprocal exchanges. Adults instinctively speak to young babies in that high-pitched, musical, gibberish language in an attempt to elicit smiles and laughter.
43
(You may have noticed that couples and pet owners can also do this.) Adults attempt to match the behaviour of the infants because babies respond to it. Sometimes, babies take the initiative and begin to spontaneously copy others around them.

These imitative behaviours are not just limited to language. Facial expressions, hand gestures, laughter and complicated actions can all be observed. Imitation signals to others that we are like them too, and we are the best imitating species on the planet. Andrew Meltzoff from the University of Washington thinks that babies really do this to
establish a ‘just like me’ relationship with the adult.
44
They are using imitation to identify others as friend or foe. The mechanism works both ways. When adults mimic the facial expressions of infants back to them, these signals are telling the baby that this person is one of them.
45

Before the child has reached their second birthday, they will copy all manner of behaviours. However, this is not slavish mimicry triggered automatically but rather an attempt by the infant to get into the mind of the adult. After watching an adult ‘fail’ to pull the end off a toy dumbbell, eighteen-month-old infants will read the true intention of the adult and complete the task they had never seen before.
46
In one study, shown in
Figure 5
, fourteen-month-olds watched an adult experimenter bend over and activate a light by pressing the button with her head (A). For some of the infants, the adult’s hands were bound by a blanket (B).

The babies were then given the light switch to play with. Infants who saw the adult whose arms were bound (B) activated the light switch with their hand because they understood that the adult was unable to use their hands. However, if they were the ones who saw that the adult’s hands were free (A), then the infants bent over and activated the button with their head too. They must have reasoned that it was important to use the head and not the hands. Infants were not simply copying the actions but rather repeating the intended goal. They had to get into the mind of the experimenter in order to work out what they wanted to achieve.
47

Older children will copy adults’ actions even when the children know the actions are pointless.
48
In one study,
preschoolers watched an adult open a clear plastic box to retrieve a toy. Some of the actions were necessary, such as opening a door on the front of the box, whereas other actions were irrelevant, such as lifting a rod that lay on the top. This behaviour is unique to humans. When presented with these sorts of sequences, children copied both the relevant and irrelevant actions whereas chimpanzees copied only those actions that were necessary to solve a task. The apes behaved in a way that was directed towards the goal of retrieving the reward, whereas for children, the goal was to faithfully copy the adult. Why would children over-imitate a pointless action? For the simple reason that children are more interested in fitting in socially with the adult than learning how to solve the task in the best possible way.
49

Figure 5: Hands-free adult activates switch in A, whereas adult’s arms are constrained in B (image courtesy of Gergely Csibra and György Gergely)

Developmental psychologist Cristine Legare at the University of Texas at Austin, thinks that this early blind imitation observed in children has profound implications for our species. Along with her anthropologist colleague Harvey Whitehouse from Oxford University, she has been looking at the origins of human rituals.
50
Rituals are the activities that bind humans together – acts with symbolic significance that demonstrate that members of a group have shared values. All cultures have rituals for various events that are typically major transitions in life – birth, adolescence, marriage and death. These events punctuate our lives and are often associated with religious beliefs and ceremonies. The rituals themselves are typically inscrutable. There is no inherent logic to them. In that sense there are no causal laws operating, but if you don’t follow the rules then the ritual is violated. There is something about carrying them out in the correct way which gives rituals
their potency. Likewise, Legare has shown that four-to six-year-olds are more likely to copy a behaviour step by step that has no obvious goal compared to one that does. In doing so, the child may be beginning to understand that there are some activities that others engage in that have no purpose but must be important precisely because they serve no obvious goal.
51

Getting into someone else’s head

You cannot directly see other people’s intentions, but you have to assume that they have them. This is called
mentalizing
– assuming that other people are intentional because they have minds. People are not random, but rather do things on purpose because they have goals that control their behaviour. In one study,
52
twelve-month-olds watched an experimenter look at one of two stuffed animal toys and exclaim, ‘Ooh, look at the kitty!’ A screen was then lowered and raised to reveal the adult holding either the kitten or the other toy. If the adult was revealed holding the other toy, the babies looked longer – they were confused by her intentions. They interpret people as doing things for a reason. If mum is looking at the sugar bowl on the table, then she is likely to pick it up but not the salt-shaker that she has not been looking at. When mum looks at and then walks over to the fridge, she does so to open it. Infants are building up an expanding repertoire of contingencies – knowing that people behave in predictable ways. When babies think that something has a mind because it appears to act as if it has purpose, they will attempt to engage in joint attention. They will even copy a robot if it appears to have a mind. By
simply interacting with a baby and responding every time the baby makes a noise or an action, the robot soon becomes an intentional agent, so that babies will actively try to engage the machine and even imitate its actions.
53

In contrast, animals do not imitate spontaneously as an attempt to initiate or engage in a social exchange. They may have the capacity for mentalizing, but invariably this is limited to situations that satisfy self-serving needs. For example, amorous male apes and monkeys will manoeuvre female partners out of the line of sight of dominant males in order to copulate surreptitiously.
54
Many animals will steal food if they believe that others cannot see the theft. All of these abilities of perspective taking are heightened when there is potential danger from a competitor. However, it is not clear that these evasive actions really involve mentalizing. I know that I can avoid the strike of a snake if I approach from out of its line of sight in much the same way that I can avoid a tumbling boulder if I coordinate my actions correctly. In neither situation do I attribute mental states. I simply observe the actions and reason about what is relevant information. To establish mentalizing, there needs to be evidence of the attribution of beliefs – states of mind that individuals hold to be true about the world in the absence of any direct evidence. If I think you have a belief, then I assume that you hold certain expectations about the world to be true.

Even then, one could attribute belief to others simply by putting yourself in their shoes. For example, we can both separately enter and exit a hotel room and I can describe what I believe you saw based on my own experience. I would reason that because we both went into the same room, you
must have seen what I saw. However, that need not be true. You might have had your eyes shut or something in the room might have changed, in which case I would be mistaken. For true mentalizing ability, you need to be able to understand that someone else might hold a different view from yours and indeed be completely wrong about the true state of the world. In other words, the litmus test for real mentalizing is the understanding that someone can hold a
false belief
.

Consider the following test. If I were to show you a confectionery box with ‘M&Ms’ written on it and ask you what is inside, then in all likelihood you would answer ‘M&Ms’. However, if I open it up to reveal pencils, then you should be a little surprised and possibly a little annoyed because you expected a chocolate treat. If I ask you what you originally thought was in the box, you would say ‘M&Ms’ because you understand that you had a false belief. This may seem trivially easy, but most three-year-olds give the wrong answer and claim that they thought there were pencils in the box.
55
It’s as if they have completely rewritten history to fit with what they now know to be true. They do not understand that they held a false belief. Understanding that someone can be mistaken is part of a capability called
theory of mind
and children operate with an increasingly complex set of assumptions about the minds of others.

If three-year-olds do not understand that they were mistaken, then it is not too surprising that they are unable to attribute false beliefs to others. If I ask you what someone else will answer when posed the same question about what’s in the box, then you understand that they, too, should answer ‘M&Ms’. You can see things from their perspective and
understand that they will also have a false belief. Again, three-year-olds give the wrong answer and say pencils. It’s as if they cannot easily take another person’s perspective.

When young children act in this self-centred view they are said to be
egocentric
because they view the world exclusively from their own perspective. If you show young children a model layout on a tabletop of a mountain range with different landmarks and buildings and then ask them to select a photograph that corresponds to the view they can see, three-year-olds correctly choose the one that matches their own perspective. However, when asked to choose the picture that corresponds to the view that someone else standing on the opposite side of the table can see, they typically choose again the photograph that matches their own.
56

Young children cannot easily formulate a mental picture of what it is like to see the world from someone else’s viewpoint. The classic demonstration of this false belief perspective involves two dolls, Sally and Anne.
57
In the Sally–Anne task, Sally has a marble that she puts in a toy chest before saying goodbye to Anne and leaving the house. Whilst she is out, Anne moves the marble from the toy chest and places it in the cupboard under the sink in the kitchen. The child is asked where Sally will look for her marble. Adults easily know that Sally will look in the original location. After all, she does not know that Anne moved the marble and she isn’t psychic! Again, three-year-olds fail the test and say Sally will look in the cupboard in the kitchen, under the sink.

BOOK: The Domesticated Brain
13.87Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Other books

Truly, Madly, Deadly by Hannah Jayne
Breaking the Bro Code by Stefanie London
Officer in Pursuit by Ranae Rose
To Have A Human by Amber Kell
Unfettered by Sasha White
Mistaken Identity by Diane Fanning
West of Paradise by Gwen Davis