You Are Not So Smart: Why You Have Too Many Friends on Facebook, Why Your Memory Is Mostly Fiction, and 46 Other Ways You're Deluding Yourself (31 page)

BOOK: You Are Not So Smart: Why You Have Too Many Friends on Facebook, Why Your Memory Is Mostly Fiction, and 46 Other Ways You're Deluding Yourself
13.49Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads
Zimbardo made sure his participants were middle-class college students with no history of violence or substance abuse. He told the guards to maintain order but didn’t give specific instructions as to how to go about it. At first, both guards and prisoners didn’t take the experiment seriously. They goofed around a bit and were slow to warm to the role-playing, but Zimbardo had the guards regularly wake up the prisoners with whistles and then count them, forcing the prisoners to recite their numbers one at time. Over time, the guards became more aggressive during these counts, more abusive, and cruel. If a prisoner broke a rule, the guards would force that person to do push-ups or place the prisoner in a closet as if it were solitary confinement. On the morning of the second day, the prisoners felt like they had endured enough and barricaded their cells with mattresses while yelling back at the pretend guards. In turn, the guards grabbed a fire extinguisher and doused the prisoners through the bars so they could force their way into the cells. They then stripped the prisoners naked, took away their beds, and began to insult and berate them. To prevent further insurrections, they allowed certain prisoners to wear clothes and sleep on beds if they maintained good, obedient behavior. They also were allowed better food and the indulgences of a toothbrush and toothpaste. After a few hours, the guards took all the privileges away from the compliant prisoners and had them switch places with the defiant ones in an attempt to scramble their minds and destroy any alliances they might have formed by creating doubt in their minds as to who was secretly cooperating with the guards. Before long, the guards were forcing the prisoners to relieve themselves in a bucket and forcing them to simulate sodomy on one another.
Zimbardo became overwhelmed by the power of the situation just as much as the students had. He started imagining himself as a warden, and when he heard rumors of a possible escape plan being hatched by the prisoners, he tried unsuccessfully to move his experiment into a real jail. Once he saw footage of the guards becoming physically violent when they thought the psychologists weren’t looking, he realized that the situation was getting out of hand. When one of his graduate students visited for the first time and recoiled in horror at the conditions the prisoners were living in, Zimbardo finally saw through her eyes that things had gone too far. On the sixth day, they ended the experiment. The prisoners rejoiced; the guards complained.
In the interviews that followed, the students who role-played as prisoners said they felt as if they had lost their identity, and that the experiment had been replaced by a real prison. They questioned their own sanity. They forgot they could leave if they only requested for the experiment to end. The guards said they were only following orders.
Remember, all of these people were just average, middle-class college students the week prior. Nothing they or anyone else knew about them suggested they were capable of such malice or conformity. It all took place in a row of offices in a building on a college campus, and everyone knew this, but the situation, the external forces, were so powerful that the participants changed into monsters and victims in just one day.
Decades later, Zimbardo would reject the U.S. government’s claims about the sadism displayed at the Abu Ghraib prison as being the result of the behavior of a few bad apples. The government was committing the fundamental attribution error, ignoring the power of the situation, turning the perpetrators into easy-to-dismiss characters. Although he doesn’t absolve those who tortured and humiliated Iraqi prisoners at Abu Ghraib, Zimbardo suggests whenever people are put into a situation like the one he created in his experiment, the same results will unfold, as they did in 2004 in the Baghdad Correctional Facility and as they have in other prisons throughout history. People are not good at heart, Zimbardo says, but because their environment encourages it. Anyone, he believes, is capable of becoming a monster if given the power and opportunity.
When you interpret your loved one’s coldness as his or her indifference to your wants and needs instead of as a reaction to stress at work or problems ricocheting in your loved one’s own heart, you’ve committed the fundamental attribution error. When you vote for someone because that person seems likable and approachable, and ignore how much of their persona is contrived for the sake of votes, it’s the same error at work. You commit it again when you assay friendliness as sexual interest, or poverty as the result of laziness. When you look for a cause for another person’s actions, you find it. Rarely, though, do you first consider how powerful the situation is. You blame the person, not the environment and the influence of the person’s peers. You do this because you would like to believe your own behavior comes strictly from within. You know this isn’t true though. You shift from introvert to extrovert, from brainiac to simpleton, from charismatic to impish—depending on where you find yourself and who is watching.
The fundamental attribution error leads to labels and assumptions about who people are, but remember first impressions are mostly incorrect. Those impressions will linger until you get to know people and understand their situation and the circumstances in which their behavior is generated. Knowing this doesn’t mean you must forgive evil, but perhaps it can help prevent it.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
A super-tight hug goes out to Erin Malone, who found my blog and believed it should exist in physical form. Through her confidence and hard work it now does. Thank you so much.
Thanks too to Patrick Mulligan, who “got it” early on and then slashed and questioned the original manuscript until it made sense. I’m fortunate to have had such a plugged-in editor.
Boundless thanks go out to my wife, Amanda, who read this book as it came together and kept it from going off the rails many times.
In many ways this book and the blog from which it sprang started in a psychology class I took seven years after graduating from high school.
After getting married, my wife and I sold all our possessions and traveled to Germany for no other reason than to see what would happen. We had both gone to a tiny school in a small town in Mississippi, and we both worked the sort of jobs that went along with those foundations—waiting tables, construction, selling coats. Escaping into a strange adventure made a lot of sense at the time. As tramps abroad, we were shocked at not only how naive we were, but how uneducated. We swore to each other when we got back to the States we would get college degrees.
One of our first college experiences was an incredibly challenging and life-changing course—Introduction to Psychology, taught by one extraordinary teacher, Jean Edwards.
Edwards’s class wasn’t like the other courses. Nothing about it was remedial. She came every day with a laptop and a projector and used videos, photos, animations, and diagrams to detail the intricacies of how the mind worked. The textbook was an afterthought, a supplement. In her class, she used presentations labored over for years to boggle our minds and shake us out of our delusions. She made us stand up and perform, she put us in and out of groups, she pointed at our faces and made us talk. When the tests landed on our desks, there was no memorization, there were no word banks. Every question was a puzzle that required a deep understanding of the material to unfurl and solve. Once we went on to a full university, my wife and I were astonished to find no course ever compared to hers.
In one class, she asked us to imagine a man who woke up every day and wrapped his entire body in newspaper before putting on his clothes. He worked hard, provided for his family, and hurt no one. At the end of the day, he would discreetly remove his wrappings before going to bed. She then asked, “Is this person crazy?” For an hour, the class argued about it. Most people’s knee-jerk reaction was “Yeah, obviously.” She nursed the conversation, pointing out our ignorance, asking us to examine our own quirks and neurotic habits. By the end of the class the students had reached a consensus: The newspaper man was as deluded as the rest of us, and therefore not crazy.
Every class with Edwards was revelatory, not just because of the overwhelming number of eye-opening facts and epiphanies, but also because she showed people like me and my wife there were kindred spirits out there. She had no problem losing an hour of her day to an after-class conversation, and she was always ready to subvert the norms and expectations of her pupils and peers. She made it safe and respectable to be different, and she provided her students with a role model they didn’t know was an option before meeting her. She was a smart, successful, professional woman who questioned everything and dared you to join her. This book, and the blog that led to it, started in that class.
So, I thank you, Jean Edwards. You changed my life and tilted my view of the world permanently.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Introduction
Nyhan, B., & Reifler, J. (2010). When corrections fail: the persistence of political misperceptions.
Political Behavior
32(2), 303–330.
Munro, G. D., & Ditto, P. H. (1997, June). Biased assimilation, attitude polarization, and affect in reactions to stereotype-relevant scientific information.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
23(6)
,
636–653.
Munro, G. (2010). The scientific impotence excuse: discounting belief-threatening scientific abstracts.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology
40(3), 579–600.
Priming
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999, July). The unbearable automaticity of being.
American Psychologist
54(7)
,
462–479.
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
71(2), 230–244.
Bargh, J. A., & Wyer, R. S. (1997).
The automaticity of everyday life.
London: Psychology Press.
Carey, B. (2007, July 31). Who’s minding the mind?
New York Times.
Friedman, R., & Elliot, A. J. (2008, November). Exploring the influence of sports drink exposure on physical endurance, Original Research Article.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise
9(6), 749–759.
Kay, A. C., Wheeler, S. C., Bargh, J. A., & Ross, L. (2004). Material priming: The influence of mundane physical objects on situational construal and competitive behavioral choice.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes
95, 83–96.
Zhong, C., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: threatened morality and physical cleansing.
Science
313(5792), 1451–1452.
Confabulation
Fiala, B., & Nichols, S. (2009). Confabulation, confidence, and introspection.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
32(2), 144–145.
Hirstein, W. (2005).
Brain fiction: Self-deception and the riddle of confabulation.
Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Joseph, R. (1986). Confabulation and delusional denial: Frontal lobe and lateralized influences.
Journal of Clinical Psychology
42, 845–860.
Mandler, G. (1985).
Cognitive psychology: An essay in cognitive science.
Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977, May). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes.
Psychological Review
84(3), 231–259.
Pantel, J. (2006, March). Brain fiction: Self-deception and the riddle of confabulation: Review.
American Journal of Psychiatry
163, 559.
Ramachandran, V. S. (2009, January 10).
Self-awareness: The last frontier.
Retrieved December 2010 from
http://integral-options.blogspot.com/2009/01/vs-ramachandran-self-awareness-last.html
.
Sapolsky, R. (1995, November 1). Ego boundaries, or the fit of my father’s shirt: A neuroscientist racks his brains to find where one person ends and another begins.
Discover.
Retrieved December 2010 from
http://discovermagazine.com/1995/nov/egoboundariesort586
.
Zaidel, E., Zaidel, D. W., & Bogen, J. E. (n.d.).
The split brain.
Retrieved December 2010 from
http://www.its.caltech.edu/~jbogen/text/ref130.htm
.
Confirmation Bias
Knobloch-Westerwick, S., & Meng, J. (2009, June). Looking the other way: Selective exposure to attitude-consistent and counter attitudinal political information.
Communication Research
36(3), 426–448.
Krebs, V. (2008).
New Political Patterns.
Retrieved December 2010 from
http://www.orgnet.com/divided.html
.
Krebs, V. (2000, March). Working in the connected world: Book network.
IHRIM Journal,
87–90.
LaMarre, H. L., Landreville, K. D., & Beam, M. A. (2009, April). The irony of satire: Political ideology and the motivation to see what you want to see in The Colbert Report.
The International Journal of Press/Politics
14(2), 212–231.
Marks, M. J., & Fraley, R. C. (2006, January). Confirmation bias and the sexual double standard.
Sex Roles
54(1/2), 19–26.
Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises.
Review of General Psychology
2(2), 175–220.
Owad, T. (2006, January 4).
Data mining 101: Finding subversives with Amazon wish lists.
Retrieved December 2010 from
http://www.applefritter.com/bannedbooks
.
Owings, J. (2009, February 24).
Confirmation bias and the internet.
Retrieved December 2010 from
http://www.justinowings.com/b/index.php/me/confirmation-bias-and-the-internet
.
Pratchett, T. (2000).
The truth: A novel of Discworld.
New York: HarperCollins.
Snyder, M., & Cantor, N. (1998). Understanding personality and social behavior: A functionalist strategy. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.).
The handbook of social psychology: Vol. 1.
(4th ed., pp. 635–679). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
BOOK: You Are Not So Smart: Why You Have Too Many Friends on Facebook, Why Your Memory Is Mostly Fiction, and 46 Other Ways You're Deluding Yourself
13.49Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Other books

Noir by Jacqueline Garlick
Fifty Shades Effed by Torcivia, Phil
Sealed With a Kiss by Gwynne Forster
Traveling Soul by Todd Mayfield
The Lesson by Welch, Virginia
Lady Parts by Andrea Martin
Naked Came the Manatee by Brian Antoni, Dave Barry, Edna Buchanan, Tananarive Due, James W. Hall, Vicki Hendricks, Carl Hiaasen, Elmore Leonard, Paul Levine
O, Juliet by Robin Maxwell